[Edu-sig] Observations from the Northwest Science Expo
Thomas O'Connor
toconnor@vcd.hp.com
Thu, 19 Apr 2001 15:39:18 -0700
On March 12 of this year, I participate as a middle school judge for
the Northwest Science Expo held on the campus of Portland State
University, in Portland Oregon. I was assigned to the team evaluating
middle school behavioral science projects along with 8 other
scientists and engineers. We evaluated 21 student projects.
This was a great group of kids and I want to be clear that this is in
no way a criticism of any of their work. I believe that the judges
took much effort to value and encourage the students. But as is often
the case, a world view can be shattered by an example of something
outstanding, which then shows all else to be mediocre. I had one of
those experiences at NWSE, and it relates to the use of computers in
education.
I'd be interested in any comments anyone in this group has to offer.
The outstanding example in this case was a project put together by
young lady in the fourth grade (who was, by the way competing against
6-8th graders). Her project investigated the effects of varying
light/dark cycle periods on bean plant growth. Her experimental
design and investigation was reasonably thorough, particularly for
someone her age, but not outstanding by comparison to those she was
competing with. What struck me as outstanding however, was how she
related to her data.
When I asked what conclusions she drew from her experiment, she made
an expansive hand motion, one of those "it should be clearly obvious
to the most casual observer," gestures towards her hand drawn data plots.
With wide eyed excitement, she explained how her data clearly proved
her original thesis incorrect. It was clear that she had fully
internalized her data. In this, she stood apart from here peers.
By contrast, most other student I spoke with related to their graphs
and charts as fuzzy abstract representations of their data. Some
examples:
* More than half of the students produced three-dimensional bar
charts to display data. Unfortunately, only one student was able to
accurately read their bar charts. The others failed to account for
the parallax between the bar and the back of the graph which
represented the X scale.
* One young man was analyzing the effect of color on mood. But when
he reported his results, his graph was color coded for color. In
other words, his red bar represented the color black, the pink bar
represented yellow, the yellow bar represented green. When asked
about this, he said his software wouldn't let him pick the colors of
the bars in his bar-chart.
* An 8th grader noted that her results did not vary significantly
from what would have been predicted from a purely random sampling.
This was a very enlightened analysis, and the judges all gave her
credit for attempting to utilize good statistical analysis. But the
statistical theory she was attempting to utilize was clearly well
beyond her knowledge and skill level. When asked why so much of her
her project display was focused on the statistical analysis of her
data when she admitted that she didn't really understand it, she
stated, "well, Excel did all the math for me."
* A sixth grader did a project to determine which parent the gene
for red hair was inherited from. Her knowledge of genetics was
impressive, particularly for her age. But the computer generated
graphs she displayed conveyed no meaning to any of the judges. Had
she displayed and focused on the genealogy charts she had stashed in
the back of her project notebook, she probably would have won an
award.
* There were two students who had a very good project on luminescent
solutions and the effects of solution temperature on luminescence.
While their presentation was very good, what I found most notable in
speaking with them was the rate of change graph they had scribbled
on the back of a piece of yellow legal pad to explain the cascade
effect they had observed. Their scribbled graph conveyed more
meaning than all their beautiful computer generated charts.
Questions to ponder:
--------------------
* Does the use of the computer to generate charts enhance the
synthesis of data, or might it actually hinder it?
In this case, the fourth grader who was determining environmental
effects on plant growth collected her data by drawing points on her
graph each day during the experiment. At the science fair, she was
able to articulate the meaning of that data better than other middle
school students I spoke with. The only other students who showed
equivalent understanding did so by utilizing a scribbled graph drawn
on the back of a legal pad.
So is there a correlation between the manual plotting of data and
the cognitive interpretation of that data?
* Gerald E. Jones in "How to LIE with CHARTS," on the "old
fassion" approach to charting mused:
Having such crude tools might have forced those early
chart-makers into slower thought processes. It is
conceivable that they actually pondered carefully
composition--maybe even the content!--of those
pathetically simple charts and graphs. Can it be that
in their technological poverty they achieved a higher
level of consciousness? Did they actually come to grasp
the meaning of their graphic creations? [1]
With the proliferation of three-dimensional bar that even the
creators can not accurately read, is the real meaning of the data
being masked by the visual presentation?
* I know from experience that I can take someone who is generally
computer phobic but who has a good understanding of statistics, sit
them down in front of Excel, and within a few hours they can be using it
productively. On the other hand, as I observed, an 8th grader who
knows Excel inside and out, cannot do meaningful statistics.
So why do so many people insist that schools teach Excel, or Word,
or Powerpoint for example? Shouldn't the real focus be on teaching
number theory, problem solving, language composition and cognitive
perception?
I'm beginning to wonder how much benefit computers actually add to
the learning process? I'd welcome a discussion on the topic by those
who are effectively utilizing computers in their curriculum today.
Thanks for the bandwidth.
Tom O.
----- Thomas O'Connor toconnor@vcd.hp.com
Hewlett Packard, Vancouver Washington
Phone: (360) 212-5031
Telnet: 212-5031
[1] "How to LIE with CHARTS," Gerald E. Jones, Sybex, 1995, p. XVII.
See also:
"The Visual Display of Quantative Information," Edward Tufte, Graphics
Press, 1983
----- Thomas O'Connor toconnor@vcd.hp.com
Hewlett Packard, Vancouver Washington
Phone: (360) 212-5031
Telnet: 212-5031